Anyone who reads is bound to wonder, at least occasionally, about how those funny squiggles on a page magically turn into "Bare ruined choirs, where late the sweet birds sang" or "After a while I went out and left the hospital and walked back to the hotel in the rain." Where did this unlikely skill called reading come from? What happens in our brain when our eyes scan a line of type? Why do some of us, or some of our children, find it difficult to process the visual information held in words?
In Proust and the Squid, Maryanne Wolf, a professor at Tufts University and director of its Center for Reading and Language Research, offers explanations for all these questions, but with an emphasis that is "more biological and cognitive than cultural-historical." This means that Wolf focuses on the physiological character of the human brain, which holds at its disposal "three ingenious design principles: the capacity to make new connections among older structures; the capacity to form areas of exquisitely precise specialization for recognizing patterns in information, and the ability to learn to recruit and connect information from these areas automatically." These "design principles" provide the neuronal foundation of reading, and Wolf spends half her book explaining the evolution and minutiae of this "reading brain."
Nearly all this material makes for very hard slogging, even though Proust and the Squid is confidently described as the author's "first book for the general public." (The catchy but utterly uninformative title, by the way, refers to the novelist's impressionistic thoughts about childhood reading and a scientist's use of the squid brain for neurological research.) A work of popularization needs a light clear style, lots of anecdotes and some plot or story line that moves along at a good clip. At times, Wolf makes a stab at including some human-interest element or personal example, but all too soon she reverts to her normal prose, which is austere, technical and, finally, wearisome:
"In a pathbreaking meta-analysis of twenty-five imaging studies of different languages, cognitive scientists from the University of Pittsburgh found three great common regions used differentially across writing systems. In the first, the occipital-temporal area (which includes the hypothesized locus of 'neuronal recycling' for literacy), we become proficient visual specialists in whatever script we read. In the second, the frontal region around Broca's area, we become specialists in two different ways -- for phonemes in words and for their meanings. In the third, the multifunction region spanning the upper temporal lobes and the lower, adjacent parietal lobes, we recruit additional areas that help to process multiple elements of sounds and meanings, which are particularly important for alphabetic and syllabary systems."
Out of context such prose sounds perfectly dreadful -- and in context sadly characteristic of the writing in professional journals, no matter what the field. In fact, everything Wolf says makes sense, the specialized terms she uses have been previously defined, and there are line illustrations on a facing page. Nonetheless, such technical onslaughts are extremely tiring to read, and Wolf seldom lets up on the information-rich barrage for very long. At different points she does quote passages from Proust and George Eliot, but even these two great novelists are hardly what you'd call sprightly, and they merely add their own specific gravity to already forbidding pages.
In the second half of the book, Wolf examines the reading difficulties generally subsumed under the term dyslexia. We learn that one of her sons suffers from this disability, that there are various forms and theories about its origin and character, that it can sometimes result in a special talent for fields that emphasize pattern and spatial creativity (such as art, design and engineering) and that "programs which systematically and explicitly teach young readers phoneme awareness and grapheme-phoneme correspondence are far more successful in dealing with reading disabilities than other programs." As this last sentence makes evident, no relief awaits the once-eager reader who by this point has begun to wonder if he could be suffering from a sudden case of adult-onset dyslexia.
Despite Wolf's failure to write a truly popular book, she clearly does know her stuff, and those professionally involved with the teaching of reading might be more patient than I. In particular, she addresses the special needs of children raised in cultures where standard English isn't the dominant language, and she speculates, with real concern, about the impact of computer culture on the "reading brain." Dyslexia has taught her that humans were never genetically designed to read, and this peculiar technique of sustained mental attention could be reduced, reconfigured or even lost in the rising digital age:
"Will unguided information lead to an illusion of knowledge, and thus curtail the more difficult, time-consuming, critical thought processes that lead to knowledge itself? Will the split-second immediacy of information gained from a search engine and the sheer volume of what is available derail the slower, more deliberative processes that deepen our understanding of complex concepts, of another's inner thought processes, and of our own consciousness?"
Wolf never fully answers these questions, though they strike me as the basis for a much needed book. Still, like any parent with a child transfixed by flashing screens, she is troubled by what she observes. She urges that we "teach our children to be 'bitextual' " or 'multitextual,' able to read and analyze texts flexibly in different ways" so that our sons and daughters don't end up as mere "decoders of information," distracted from the "deeper development of their intellectual potential." Early on in Proust and the Squid, she had noted that infants and toddlers who aren't told stories by their caregivers, who aren't read to from a very early age, nearly always fail to learn to read well themselves. By implication, it may already be too late for many young people: They will never be able to read with the same thoughtfulness and comprehension as their parents. Think about that.
互联网的未来-光荣.毁灭与救赎的预言 目录 译者序引言**部分可繁殖网络的兴起和停滞**章盒子的战争第二章网络的战争第三章网络安全与自我繁殖的两难第二部分停滞之...
《惊隐诗社研究》内容简介:明清交替之际,是文人结社活动最为活跃的一个时期,曾经涌现过许许多多的社团。清朝初年以江苏吴江为中
InDesign CC实战从入门到精通-(全彩版) 本书特色 InDesign是当今流行的专业排版软件,被广泛应用于平面设计、印刷出版、数字媒体等诸多领域。本书...
《人间词话(高中部分)》内容简介:本书是著名学者王国维先生的文学批评著作。词话,和“诗话”一样,是中国古代文学批评的一种文
电脑维修与技巧888问 内容简介 电脑已经成为人们生活中不可缺少的一部分。虽然大家都在使用电脑,但不是每个人都能够使用得得心应手,当电脑出故障时,许多人就束手无...
Java Web从入门到精通(配光盘)(软件开发视频大讲堂) 本书特色 “软件开发视频大讲堂”丛书系清华社“视频大讲堂”重点大系之一。该大系包括多个子系列,每个...
《MATLAB小波分析超级学习手册》内容简介:本书对小波分析在MATLAB中的应用进行了详细的介绍,全书以小波为主题展开叙述,不仅对小
《你看起来好像……我爱你》内容简介:这是一本写给普通人了解AI的趣味科普,它还有超级可爱的漫画!这本书“非常易读、有料,而且
高鹏、赵培、陈庆涛编著的《3G技术问答(第二版)》从3G空中接口的四个主流标准(TD-SCDMA、WCDMA、cdma2000和WiMAX)的内容中精
《建筑师与设计师视觉笔记》旨在鼓励视觉表达,其理念基于这样一种观点,即视觉修养与文字修养同等重要。为了开拓表达视觉信息的
WordPress爱好者的福音,由WordPress团队的美女成员LisaSabin-Wilson编著的WordPressForDummies(WordPres...
Combinatorylogicandlambda-calculus,originallydevisedinthe1920s,havesincedevelope...
《道德经与领导力》内容简介:本书将《道德经》与实际案例相结合,通过详细阐述《道德经》的真髓来告诉高级领导者如何处世,其中包
《微机原理及应用》是教育科学“十五”国家规划课题研究成果。全书共13章,包括:基础知识,微型计算机概论,8086/8088指令系统与
《小顾聊绘画(壹)》内容简介:梵高、莫奈等9位艺术大家的人生长廊,高清原彩画作,妙趣横生的艺术解读! 艺术没有那么高不可攀!
ASP.NET是由Microsoft公司推出的新一代Web开发构架。开发人员可以通过ASP.NET实现动态网站的开发,包括开发Web应用程序和Web服务
《设计模式之禅》内容简介:全书共分为四部分,第一部分从原理的角度阐述了面向对象程序设计的6大原则;第二部生动地讲解和剖析了2
《生育制度》内容简介:《生育制度》是费孝通1946年根据他在西南联大和云南大学任教时的讲义整理而成,围绕“家庭三角”这一核心议
MATLAB 优化算法案例分析与应用 本书特色 本书全面而系统地介绍了matlab算法和案例应用,涉及面广,从基本操作到高级算法应用,几乎涵盖matlab算法的...
《金融风险管理(第二版)》内容简介:随着金融自由化、全球化的发展以及层出不穷的金融创新,金融机构所处的风险环境日益复杂,金